From New
Scientist Magazine – 28th April 2016
Why are religions so judgemental? Ask evolution
Nicolas Baumard
The rise of moralising religions like Christianity
can be explained by evolution – and so can their eventual downfall, says
evolutionary psychologist Nicolas Baumard
Forgive me, father |
WHEN
Jesus of Nazareth died on the cross, leaving behind a few dozen followers in a
remote province of the Roman Empire, few would have guessed that 350 years
later Christianity would be the official religion of the Roman Empire and would
go on to become the most widely practised religion in the world.
Christianity's
success is often attributed to its supposedly unique message. Unlike earlier
religions, it exhorted people to be good and promised to reward them for their
goodness in the afterlife. That is still how most people conceptualise the
Christian message: helping others, working hard, controlling one's sexuality
and believing that people who don't do so will be punished. In other words, a
moralising religion.
It is
true that before Christianity, most religions did not place a high value on
morality. The Greco-Roman religions, for example, were materialistic, mostly
concerned with rituals, sacrifices and other ways of begging favours from their
various divinities.
But
Christ's message was not actually new. In Homer's time, the 8th century BC, the
Greeks believed that when people died they all, good and bad, went to Hades.
From the 5th century BC on, Greeks started to believe that the dead were judged
in Hades according to their deeds during life. Judaism, too, began to
incorporate beliefs about moral punishment in the afterlife.
Roman religions were materialistic rather than moralising |
Parallel developments ccurred in societal values. While the heroes of Homer's Iliad were openly polygamous and unfaithful, fidelity and monogamy started to be promoted towards the end of the 1st century BC. Where Achilles and Agamemnon were quick-tempered, sexually rapacious and arrogant, the moralists of the Roman Empire started to defend an ethics of asceticism and modesty.
Christianity,
then, was part of a wave of new religions that emerged more than 2000 years
ago. What happened to make materialistic religions transform into moralising
ones?
Social
scientists sometimes explain this by arguing that moralising religions promote
cooperation, which would have given the societies that adopted them a
competitive advantage. Religion was a sort of "social glue" that
bound societies together as they grew beyond the point where everyone was
related and family ties stopped people from freeloading. That seems a plausible
explanation – except for the fact that moralising religions did not arise until
quite late in human history, long after the rise of large-scale societies in
Egypt and Sumeria.
Recent
research in behavioural ecology and experimental psychology suggests a
different answer. This work, known as life history theory, finds that organisms
are endowed with evolved programmes that modulate their behaviour according to
their environment. In a harsh and unpredictable environment, when resources are
scarce and mortality is high, organisms adopt a "fast life strategy".
They mature and reproduce earlier, invest less in offspring and pair-bonding,
and are impulsive and aggressive.
For
instance, starlings placed in a highly competitive environment for
a few days during their development go on to invest less in physical
maintenance, with a lower body weight and reduced levels of DNA repair. They
also develop a fast psychology, preferring immediate rewards over riskier but
potentially more profitable investments in foraging. From an evolutionary
perspective this makes sense: if you could die at any time, your best shot of
passing on your genes is to grab what you can.
In
contrast, in a more favourable and predictable environment, organisms switch to
a slow life strategy. They mature and reproduce later, invest more in offspring
and pair-bonding, and become more patient and more forgiving.
The same
ability to switch also exists in humans, and an abundant stream of research
shows that as the environment gets better, individuals start investing more in
their family and romantic relationships, and become less impulsive and less
aggressive (New Scientist, 17
July 2010, p 40). For example, women from more affluent
neighbourhoods are likely to have babies at a later age. They have larger
babies and breastfeed more, both of which make it more difficult to get
pregnant again.
This is
strikingly similar to what happened in the eastern Mediterranean 2500 years
ago. Around that time, energy use per capita – a good proxy for affluence –
rose from the 15,000 calories per day typically seen in Egyptian and Sumerian
civilisations to more than 20,000 calories per day. And as people became more
affluent and society more stable and predictable, their slow strategy kicked
in.
At the
same time, we see the invention and spread of moralising religions. Are the two
connected?
I think
so. Consider the fact that for a long time, the switch from fast to slow
strategy was restricted to only the most affluent members of the population.
Everybody else was still living fast and dying young, and the elite were none
too happy about it.
The same
idea could also explain the gradual decline of moralising religion in wealthier
parts of the world such as Western Europe and the northern parts of North America.
As more and more people become affluent and adopt a slow strategy, the need to
morally condemn fast strategies decreases, and with it the benefit of holding
religious beliefs that justify doing so.
If this
is true, and our environment continues to improve, then like the Greco-Roman
religions before them, Christianity and other moralising religions could
eventually vanish.
---
This article appeared in print under the headline
"Morality tale"
Nicolas Baumard is an evolutionary psychologist at the École
Normale Supérieure in Paris
No comments:
Post a Comment